Publication:
The link between early iconic gesture comprehension and receptive language

dc.contributor.coauthorOzer, Demet
dc.contributor.coauthorAktan-Erciyes, Asli
dc.contributor.coauthorFurman, Reyhan
dc.contributor.coauthorDemir-Lira, O. Ece
dc.contributor.coauthorOzcaliskan, Seyda
dc.contributor.departmentDepartment of Psychology
dc.contributor.departmentGraduate School of Social Sciences and Humanities
dc.contributor.kuauthorDoğan, Işıl
dc.contributor.kuauthorGöksun, Tilbe
dc.contributor.schoolcollegeinstituteCollege of Social Sciences and Humanities
dc.contributor.schoolcollegeinstituteGRADUATE SCHOOL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES AND HUMANITIES
dc.date.accessioned2025-03-06T20:58:36Z
dc.date.issued2024
dc.description.abstractChildren comprehend iconic gestures relatively later than deictic gestures. Previous research with English-learning children indicated that they could comprehend iconic gestures at 26 months, a pattern whose extension to other languages is not yet known. The present study examined Turkish-learning children's iconic gesture comprehension and its relation to their receptive vocabulary knowledge. Turkish-learning children between the ages of 22- and 30-month-olds (N = 92, M = 25.6 months, SD = 1.6;51 girls) completed a gesture comprehension task in which they were asked to choose the correct picture that matched the experimenter's speech and iconic gestures. They were also administered a standardized receptive vocabulary test. Children's performance in the gesture comprehension task increased with age, which was also related to their receptive vocabulary knowledge. When children were categorized into younger and older age groups based on the median age (i.e., 26 months-the age at which iconic gesture comprehension was present for English-learning children), only the older group performed at chance level in the task. At the same time, receptive vocabulary was positively related to gesture comprehension for younger but not older children. These findings suggest a shift in iconic gesture comprehension at around 26 months and indicate a possible link between receptive vocabulary knowledge and iconic gesture comprehension, particularly for children younger than 26 months.
dc.description.indexedbyWOS
dc.description.indexedbyScopus
dc.description.publisherscopeInternational
dc.description.sponsoredbyTubitakEuN/A
dc.description.sponsorshipJames S. McDonnell Foundation, Grant/Award Number: 220020510
dc.identifier.doi10.1002/icd.2552
dc.identifier.eissn1522-7219
dc.identifier.grantnoJames S. McDonnell Foundation [220020510]
dc.identifier.issn1522-7227
dc.identifier.issue6
dc.identifier.quartileQ2
dc.identifier.scopus2-s2.0-85208417693
dc.identifier.urihttps://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2552
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/20.500.14288/27512
dc.identifier.volume33
dc.identifier.wos1357579700001
dc.keywordsIconic gesture comprehension
dc.keywordsIndividual differences
dc.keywordsReceptive vocabulary
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherWiley
dc.relation.ispartofINFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT
dc.subjectPsychology
dc.titleThe link between early iconic gesture comprehension and receptive language
dc.typeJournal Article
dspace.entity.typePublication
local.publication.orgunit1College of Social Sciences and Humanities
local.publication.orgunit1GRADUATE SCHOOL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES AND HUMANITIES
local.publication.orgunit2Department of Psychology
local.publication.orgunit2Graduate School of Social Sciences and Humanities
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublicationd5fc0361-3a0a-4b96-bf2e-5cd6b2b0b08c
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublicatione192fff1-4efe-45a7-ab71-30233fc185a9
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublication.latestForDiscoveryd5fc0361-3a0a-4b96-bf2e-5cd6b2b0b08c
relation.isParentOrgUnitOfPublication3f7621e3-0d26-42c2-af64-58a329522794
relation.isParentOrgUnitOfPublicationc5c9bf5f-4655-411c-a602-0d68f2e2ad88
relation.isParentOrgUnitOfPublication.latestForDiscovery3f7621e3-0d26-42c2-af64-58a329522794

Files