Publication: Peer persuasion strategies during rule following in 4-to 6-year-olds
dc.contributor.coauthor | Kumkale, Gökçe Tarcan | |
dc.contributor.department | N/A | |
dc.contributor.department | Department of Psychology | |
dc.contributor.kuauthor | Şen, Hilal Harma | |
dc.contributor.kuauthor | Küntay, Aylin C. | |
dc.contributor.kuprofile | PhD Student | |
dc.contributor.kuprofile | Faculty Member | |
dc.contributor.other | Department of Psychology | |
dc.contributor.schoolcollegeinstitute | Graduate School of Social Sciences and Humanities | |
dc.contributor.schoolcollegeinstitute | College of Social Sciences and Humanities | |
dc.contributor.yokid | 308928 | |
dc.contributor.yokid | 178879 | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2024-11-09T23:12:07Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2021 | |
dc.description.abstract | Despite being a ubiquitous form of communication, persuasive communication between children received relatively scant research attention. We used a novel approach to study children's persuasive interactions in a semi-naturalistic paradigm where dyads of 4- to 6-year-old children were asked to follow a rule together. In this context, we observed (a) the frequency and types of strategies children spontaneously used to persuade their partners to abide by the rule, (b) compliance behavior following the persuasion attempt, and (c) strategy change following a failed attempt to persuade. We coded children's persuasive strategies as gentle control, negative control, and non-engagement. Children tended to use gentle control and negative control strategies to a greater extent than non-engagement strategies to prevent their partner's potential rule violation. However, after a violation occurred, children most frequently displayed non-engagement. Gentle control brought about more compliance than the other strategies. Children tended to maintain their persuasive strategy of negative control and non-engagement, even though these were mostly ineffective in achieving compliance. These results show that 4- to 6-year-old children spontaneously and flexibly utilized persuasive strategies with peers to abide by a rule. | |
dc.description.indexedby | WoS | |
dc.description.indexedby | Scopus | |
dc.description.issue | 4 | |
dc.description.openaccess | NO | |
dc.description.publisherscope | International | |
dc.description.volume | 30 | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1111/sode.12525 | |
dc.identifier.eissn | 1467-9507 | |
dc.identifier.issn | 0961-205X | |
dc.identifier.quartile | Q3 | |
dc.identifier.scopus | 2-s2.0-85109420965 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sode.12525 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.14288/9763 | |
dc.identifier.wos | 671618300001 | |
dc.keywords | Compliance | |
dc.keywords | Gentle control | |
dc.keywords | Negative control | |
dc.keywords | Non-engagement | |
dc.keywords | Peer interaction | |
dc.keywords | Persuasion | |
dc.language | English | |
dc.publisher | Wiley | |
dc.source | Social Development | |
dc.subject | Psychology | |
dc.title | Peer persuasion strategies during rule following in 4-to 6-year-olds | |
dc.type | Journal Article | |
dspace.entity.type | Publication | |
local.contributor.authorid | 0000-0002-9877-5108 | |
local.contributor.authorid | 0000-0001-9057-7556 | |
local.contributor.kuauthor | Şen, Hilal Harma | |
local.contributor.kuauthor | Küntay, Aylin C. | |
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublication | d5fc0361-3a0a-4b96-bf2e-5cd6b2b0b08c | |
relation.isOrgUnitOfPublication.latestForDiscovery | d5fc0361-3a0a-4b96-bf2e-5cd6b2b0b08c |